Learning Targets: I can use words, phrases, and clauses as well as varied syntax to link the major sections of the text, create cohesion, and clarify the relationships between claim(s) and reasons.
I can establish and maintain a formal style and objective tone while attending to the norms
I can cite strong and thorough textual evidence to support analysis of what the text says explicitly as well as inferences drawn from the text, including determining where the text leaves matters uncertain.and conventions of the discipline in which they are writing.
Essential question: Are tabloids the result of evolution?
Class directions:
Please read the following excerpt from
Of Tabloids and Family Secrets: The Evolutionary
Psychology of Gossip
Note the two salient points made by the authors.
In a couple of well-written paragraphs (approximately 250 words and including textual evidence), discuss the value of their research in terms of understanding human interactions.
Please send along, as usual.
Note the two salient points made by the authors.
In a couple of well-written paragraphs (approximately 250 words and including textual evidence), discuss the value of their research in terms of understanding human interactions.
Please send along, as usual.
FRANCIS T.
MCANDREW AND MEGAN A. MILENKOVIC Knox College
Two
experiments tested hypotheses about gossip derived from an evolutionary
perspective.
In the first
experiment, 128 people ranging in age from 17 to 62 years ranked the interest
value of 12 tabloid stories about celebrities differing in age and gender. In
the second experiment, 83 college students ranked the interest value and
likelihood of spreading gossip about male or female professors, relatives,
friends, acquaintances, or strangers based on 12 different gossip scenarios.
The results of these experiments confirmed a consistent pattern of interest in
gossip marked by a preference for information about others of the same age and
gender. Exploitable information in the form of damaging, negative news about
nonallies and positive news about allies was especially prized and likely to be
passed on. The findings confirm that gossip can serve as a strategy of status
enhancement and function in the interests of individuals, and that it does not
just function as a means of social control within groups.
The tendency
to gossip is at the heart of the social life of many people, and most casual
conversations are concerned with matters of social importance (Dunbar, 1996).
All available historical information and cross-cultural data suggest that this
has always been the case (Barkow, 1992; Cox, 1970; Lee, 1990; McPherson, 1991;
Paine, 1967; Schein, 1994).
Levin and
Arluke (1987), among others, proposed that gossip is universal because it is
psychologically and socially useful. Within the field of social psychology, the
few attempts to tie gossip into a larger body of theory have typically described
it as a form of social learning or social comparison (Levin & Arluke, 1987;
Morreal, 1994; Suls, 1977). In other words, gossip is a way to get information about
others that can give us guidance for leading our own lives or increased feelings
of self-worth through comparison with others (Levin & Arluke, 1987). Gossip
can be an especially appealing way to acquire social-comparison information because
it is so indirect and nonthreatening. Comparing oneself to high-rankingpeople
may be important in self-evaluation, but doing so directly and publicly could
be risky and threaten the standing that one already has (Suls,1977). Thinking
about gossip as a form of social comparison can be even moreuseful if the
connection between social-comparison processes and gossip is embedded in a
larger theoretical context that will help to make greater sense of both. It
seems as if the perspective offered by evolutionary psychology might provide
such a unifying principle.
The
available evidence suggests that our ancestors lived their lives as members of
small cooperative groups that were in competition with other relatively small
groups (Dunbar, 1996; Lewin, 1993; Tooby & DeVore, 1987). To make matters
more complicated, it was not only necessary to cooperate with in-group members
for success against out-groups; in-group competition was unavoidable insofar as
it was necessary to divide resources among the group members (Krebs &
Denton, 1997). Living in such groups, our ancestors faced a number of
consistent adaptive problems that were social in nature: obtaining a
reproductively valuable mate; and successfully managing friendships, alliances,
and family relationships (Shackelford, 1997). The social intelligence needed
for success in this early environment required an ability to predict and
influence the behavior of others (Alexander, 1979; Barkow, 1989, 1992;
Humphrey, 1983). Any process that provided fitness-relevant information would
have been strongly selected for, and an irresistible interest in gossip
(especially when coupled with a tendency to engage in social comparison) would
have been handy indeed. Thus, a strong drive to quickly gather and exchange
social information for the management of social alliances would have bestowed a
tremendous advantage on those individuals with such predispositions.
Does gossip play a role in natural
selection?
Boehm (1999)
further proposed that gossip could serve as a leveling mechanism for
neutralizing the dominance tendencies of others, a “stealthy activity by which
other people’s moral dossiers are constantly reviewed” (p. 73). Boehm believes
that small-scale foraging societies, such as those typical during human prehistory,
emphasized an egalitarianism that suppressed internal competition and promoted
consensus seeking in a way that reduced within-group selection pressures and
increased the importance of between-group differences in the selection process. These social pressures discouraged free
riders and cheaters and encouraged altruists (Boehm, 1997). He also believes
that such egalitarian societies were necessary because of the relatively equal
and unstable balance of power among individuals with access to weapons and
shifting coalitions. In these societies, the manipulation of public opinion
through gossip, ridicule, and ostracism became a key way of keeping potentially
dominant group members in check (Boehm, 1993).
The aspect
of gossip that is troubling to many is that it is not only a mechanism used by
groups to enforce conformity, but it can also be a strategy used by individuals
to further their own reputations and selfish interests at the expense of others
(Dunbar, 1996; Emler, 1994; Spacks, 1985). When framed under within group
selection, gossip is very much about enhancing one’s own success in social
competition (Barkow, 1989). Gossip offers a means of manipulating others’
reputations by passing on negative information about competitors or enemies, as
well as a means of detecting betrayal by others in our important relationships
(Shackelford, 1997; Spacks, 1985). According to Barkow (1992), we should be
especially interested in information about people who matter most in our lives:
rivals, mates, relatives, partners in social exchange, and high-ranking people
whose behavior can affect us. Barkow also proposes that the type of information
that we seek should be information that can affect our social standing relative
to others. Hence, information about control of resources (e.g., financial
news); sexual activity; current alliances and political dealings; and an
individual’s reputation as a reliable, trustworthy partner in social exchange
will be especially interesting to us. All of these speculations about the within-group
selective advantages of gossiping make sense and are consistent with
evolutionary thinking. However, in contrast to the aforementioned experiments
documenting the social-control functions of gossip, there has been very little
empirical evidence that gossip does in fact function to promote selfish
individual interests.
Thus, the
intense familiarity with celebrities provided by the modern media trips the
same gossip mechanisms that have evolved to keep up with the affairs of
in-group members. After all, anyone whom we see that often and know that much
about must be socially important to us. This will be especially true for
television actors in soap operas that are seen on a daily basis. In fact, it
has been documented that tabloids prefer stories about television actors who
are seen regularly to movie stars who are seen less often. These famous people
become familiar friends whose characters take on a life of their own (Levin
& Arluke, 1987). The public’s interest in these high-status members of our
social world seems insatiable; circulation of supermarket tabloids and
magazines such as People and Us run into the tens of millions per week. People
seem to be interested in almost all aspects of celebrity lives, but
unflattering stories about violations of norms or bad habits are most in
demand. Stories about ordinary people typically only make it into the tabloids
if they concern extraordinary events (Levin & Arluke, 1987).
No comments:
Post a Comment